Skip to main content
search
Behavioral Test Questions

How should I habituate the animals to experimenter handling?

By January 21, 2019October 8th, 2019No Comments

Animal handling prior to behavioral experimentation is a crucial consideration for all researchers. However, there are many factors which one must consider when deciding whether or not to habituate their animals to experimenter handling. Of important consideration are the characteristics of the animal including murine type (mouse or rat), strain, and the age of the animal.

For pre-experimentation handling, researchers must remove the mouse or rat from its cage and either simply hold, or alternatively, stroke the animal for a duration of no less than one minute at least once per day for one week prior to behavioral assays. Neonate handling follows a similar time-course, however, the animals should be removed from the cage and placed in a similarly sized area for the duration rather than be held throughout. Of importance is that handling should be done gently and not with forceful restraining. Animal restraint results in inverse outcomes to the above handling procedure and therefore negatively influences behavioral performance, especially in tasks involving exploration, socialization, and memory.

Under most circumstances, habituation to experimenter handling imbues progressive improvements in animal behavior which increase with the duration of this procedure. The most notable differences seen in handled vs. non-handled rodents relate to reductions in stress and anxiety. This reduction translates to more exploratory behavior and less inhibition in choice-making tasks and social interactions.

While pre-experimentation handling consistently results in behavioral adaptation amongst rodents, the effects of this handling are variable based on the age, sex and strain of the animal. As such, these factors are important for researchers to consider when deciding to habituate their animals to experimenter handling and also when interpreting the results of their experiments.

habituating rodents of different age group

Age

Neonates

Handling of neonatal rodents has been shown to induce an adaptive response to stress, as exhibited by reduced stress responses later in life. The protocol for neonatal handling is simply to remove the pup (or pups) from the home cage and place them in a separate small compartment. Variations on the procedure include:

  • handling time (1-15 minutes)
  • number of handling days (10-21 days)
  • isolation vs group handling (i.e. handling individual pups vs entire litters)

This handling procedure under various conditions has been shown to have a positive effect on later-life stress responses, indicating beneficial habituation to experimenter handling. This impact has been suggested as the result of hippocampal cell density. The hippocampus negatively regulates the HPA-axis, which is responsible for the biochemical stress response. Therefore, enhanced hippocampal development induced by handling has a consequently inhibitory effect on the stress response. Additional data suggest that increases in amygdala benzodiazepine receptor density, coupled with decreased levels of noradrenaline, serotonin, and dopamine also contribute to this increased resilience to stress.[1]

Despite these benefits of neonatal handling, there are potential downsides to this practice. Rats handled as neonates show impaired aversive learning/memory later in life. Specifically, the expression and extinction of both cue- and context-induced fear response memories are significantly inhibited in rats which have been handled during early life.[1] Additionally, rat handling during this period decreases social approach behaviors in adulthood and increases aggressive behavior in a social environment.[2] These impacts on learning and memory, as well as on social behavior, indicate that neonatal handling may not be ideal for researchers performing tasks concerned with these cognitive domains.

Adolescent

Habituating rats to experimenter handling during the adolescent period decreases stress-responding and consequently improves performance in certain tasks which measure anxiety-like states. For instance, handled rats spend more time in the open arms of an elevated plus maze as compared to non-handled adolescent rats. Additionally, transfer latency of escape behavior in the elevated plus maze is decreased in adolescent handled rats. This measure indicates that the handled rats show enhanced memory acquisition as compared to non-handled rats.[3]

Adult

Habituation of adult rats to experimenter handling, similarly to neonatal handling, reduces stress and anxiety and therefore may improve performance in novel environmental tasks. Across multiple strains of rat, handling has been shown to increase time spent in the center of an open field task, indicating reduced anxiogenic responding to the novel environment. Similarly, when presented with a hiding compartment to which rats recess when experiencing stress, handled rats exhibited fewer entries and less overall hidden time when they were handled by experimenters prior to testing.[4]

In addition to decreased anxiety-like behaviors, handled rats exhibit increased responding to some stimulation protocols. When given the psychomotor stimulant cocaine, for example, handled rats show significantly higher levels of locomotion than non-handled rats given the same drug.[5] These data may similarly be the consequence of reduced stress and therefore less inhibitory behavioral influence in environmental exploration. Therefore, handling of adult rats prior to behavioral testing will likely improve performance in a variety of tasks involving environmental novelty and risk-taking behavior.

Sex differences

Significant differences between the outcome of handling in male versus female rats represent an important consideration for researchers using animals of both sexes. For instance, habituating animals to experimenter handling prior to a forced swim test (FST; an indicator of depression or helplessness in murine models) decreases immobility and therefore indicates reduced levels of stress and depression in both sexes. However, stress adaptation to chronic stressors (such as repeated FTS exposure) is enhanced in handled males and decreased in handled females.[6] Similarly, in the morris water maze, handled male rats exhibit faster acquisition, indicating elevated stress-induced memory responding. This effect is not found in handled female rats.[7] Conversely, handled female rats exhibit improved learning and reversal learning in the Y-maze task, while handling of male rats imbues the opposite effect.[8]

Additionally, the expression of some genes whose regulation has been tied to key behavioral disorders such as Fragile X mental retardation shows similar sexually-differentiated regulation by handling. For example, expression of the Reelin gene in the hippocampus is persistently downregulated by handling in male rats, but not females.[9] This alteration may have profound consequences on exploratory and social behavior, as are found in disease models which similarly regulate Reelin expression. For instance, exploratory and investigative behaviors are more pronounced in handled female, but not male rats.[10]

Therefore, sex differences in handling outcomes are an important consideration for the interpretation of behavioral results in handled rats.

Strain differences

Differences in the effects of habituation to experimenter handling have been noted between different strains of murine mammals within the same species. These differences must be taken into consideration when electing to perform pre-test handling and determining the best method for doing so.

Two of the most widely used strains of laboratory rats, the inbred Lewis strain and the outbred Sprague Dawley strain, exhibit substantial differences in their response to experimenter handling. Standard protocols of 1-2 minutes of handling, three times per day for either a short period (one week) or a long period (two weeks), progressively decrease levels of stress hormones in Sprague Dawley, but not Lewis rats.[11] Decreased levels of these hormones are correlated with improved performance in many behavioral tasks, including open field exploration and elevated plus mazes.

Indeed, experiments examining the effects of habituation to experimenter handling between Sprague Dawley and another commonly used strain of laboratory rat (PVG) found increased exploratory behavior in the elevated plus maze only in pre-handled Sprague Dawley rats. Experimenter handling of the PVG rats prior to testing was found to have no influence on the behavioral performance.[12] Therefore, researchers intending to perform experiments which are influenced by states of stress and anxiety may see improved performance in Sprague Dawley rats with prolonged pre-experimentation handling which is not seen with Lewis rats.

Similar differences have been noted amongst commonly used strains of laboratory mice. Specifically, C57BL/6J and A/J have been tested under comparison protocols for responding to neonatal handling with regards to both pup behavior and maternal behavior.[13] Handling of C57BL/6J was found to progressively alter pup ultrasonic vocalizations, an early indicator of stress development. Conversely, A/J pups showed no change in this behavior following experimenter handling. Additionally, increases in body temperature which signal pup anxiety decrease over repeated handling in C57BL/6J pups over repeated handling while no such adaptation occurred in A/J pups. Dams of either strain showed increased nursing and grooming behaviors following experimenter handling, which adversely impacts sociobehavioral development in pops. Together, these findings suggest that experimenters using C57BL/6J will likely see positive behavioral adaptations with repeated pre-test handling of neonates but not handling of dams, while those using A/J mice may potentially see detrimental effects of maternal handling or see no benefit from neonatal handling alone.

Comparing the same C57BL/6J mice against two other strains of mouse often used in behavioral experimentation, 129S6 and Swiss Webster, experimenters have found similar strain differences in response to handling.[14] Following handling, all three strains show adaptation in a number of important parameters including body temperature and heart rate increases. These adaptations indicate progressively reduced stress following experimenter handling. Interestingly, 129S6 mouse exploratory behavior does not adapt to experimenter handling, while C57BL/6J and Swiss Webster both exhibit behavioral adaptation with regards to general exploration. Thus, as with the previously described findings, experimenters using the most common mouse strain, C57BL/6J, will see increased performance in behavioral assays following experimenter handling.

Conclusions

Clearly, habituation to experimenter handling has the potential to improve behavioral performance by rodents when performed prior to experimentation. Important considerations for researchers when electing to perform pre-experimentation handling include the age, sex, and strain of the animal, with outcomes varying based on all of these factors. Crucially, because handling does significantly impact behavior, this procedure must be performed consistently across all animals used in a given experiment. Finally, if using multiple strains of a mouse or rat (for example, when using a transgenic inbred mouse compared with an outbred strain), extra care must be taken in interpreting the results given that different strains respond differently with regards to habituation to experimenter handling.

References

  1. Raineki, C., Lucion, A. B., & Weinberg, J. (2014). Neonatal handling: an overview of the positive and negative effects. Developmental psychobiology, 56(8), 1613-25.
  2. Todeschin, A. S., Winkelmann-Duarte, E. C., Jacob, M. H. V., Aranda, B. C. C., Jacobs, S., Fernandes, M. C., Lucion, A. B. (2009). Effects of neonatal handling on social memory, social interaction, and number of oxytocin and vasopressin neurons in rats. Hormones and Behavior, 56(1), 93–100.
  3. Costa, R., Tamascia, M. L., Nogueira, M. D., Casarini, D. E., & Marcondes, F. K. (2012). Handling of adolescent rats improves learning and memory and decreases anxiety. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS, 51(5), 548-53.
  4. Rebouças, R. C. ., & Schmidek, W. R. (1997). Handling and Isolation in Three Strains of Rats Affect Open Field, Exploration, Hoarding and Predation. Physiology & Behavior, 62(5), 1159–1164.
  5. Maldonado, A. M., & Kirstein, C. L. (2005). Handling alters cocaine-induced activity in adolescent but not adult male rats. Physiology & behavior, 84(2), 321-6.
  6. Papaioannou, A., Gerozissis, K., Prokopiou, A., Bolaris, S., & Stylianopoulou, F. (2002). Sex differences in the effects of neonatal handling on the animal’s response to stress and the vulnerability for depressive behaviour. Behavioural Brain Research, 129(1-2), 131–139.
  7. Stamatakis, A., Stamatakis, A., Pondiki, S., Stamatakis, A., Pondiki, S., Kitraki, E., … Stylianopoulou, F. (2008). Effect of neonatal handling on adult rat spatial learning and memory following acute stress. Stress, 11(2), 148–159.
  8. Noschang, C., Krolow, R., Arcego, D. M., Toniazzo, A. P., Huffell, A. P., & Dalmaz, C. (2012). Neonatal handling affects learning, reversal learning and antioxidant enzymes activities in a sex-specific manner in rats. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(4), 285–291.
  9. Gross, C. M., Flubacher, A. , Tinnes, S. , Heyer, A. , Scheller, M. , Herpfer, I. , Berger, M. , Frotscher, M. , Lieb, K. and Haas, C. A. (2012), Early life stress stimulates hippocampal reelin gene expression in a sex‐specific manner: Evidence for corticosterone‐mediated action. Hippocampus, 22: 409-420.
  10. Fujimoto, T., Kubo, K., Nishikawa, Y., & Aou, S. (2014). Brief neonatal handling alters sexually dimorphic behaviors in adult rats. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 13(01), 61–70.
  11. Deutsch-Feldman, M., Picetti, R., Seip-Cammack, K., Zhou, Y., & Kreek, M. J. (2015). Effects of handling and vehicle injections on adrenocorticotropic and corticosterone concentrations in Sprague-Dawley compared with Lewis rats. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS, 54(1), 35-9.
  12. Schmitt, U., & Hiemke, C. (1998). Strain Differences in Open-Field and Elevated Plus-Maze Behavior of Rats Without and With Pretest Handling. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 59(4), 807–811.
  13. Hennessy, M. B., Li, J., Lowe, E. L., & Levine, S. (1980). Maternal behavior, pup vocalizations, and pup temperature changes following handling in mice of 2 inbred strains. Developmental Psychobiology, 13(6), 573–584.
  14. Van Bogaert, M. J., Groenink, L. , Oosting, R. S., Westphal, K. G., Van Der Gugten, J. and Olivier, B. (2006), Mouse strain differences in autonomic responses to stress. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5: 139-149.
Close Menu