Digit Span Test

About Digit Span Test

The Digit Sequencing Test alternatively called the Digit Span Test is a key tool for working verbal memory. Usually, the examiner reads a list of numbers as the digit sequencing, and the participant repeats them until an incorrect answer is given. In the ConductScience Digital Health  version, an audible module lists the numbers and the examiner inputs them onto the screen module. It can be modulated to digit spans of  1-100 sequences long, with forward and reverse sequencing available as features.

Digit Span Test: Introduction

Our working memory is a complicated system that allows us to process new stimuli while using old information at the same time (Conway et al., 2005). Note that usually, working memory is measured via three scales: Arithmetic, Digit Span, and Letter-Number Sequencing (Lovett et al., 2011).

Verbal working memory has been a primary focus of research. That’s not surprising: the verbal working memory plays a crucial role in one’s everyday life. From recalling a phone number to learning a new language, the verbal working memory is a leading factor in people’s cognitive functioning. It’s accepted that the verbal working memory consists of the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad.

The Digit Sequencing or Digit Span test is one of the main tools developed to measure one’s verbal working memory. The test was originally designed to test working memory and attention, as part of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale. Usually, the examiner reads a list of numbers, and the participant repeats them until an incorrect answer is given (Blackburn, 1957). Sometimes the participant is asked to repeat the sequence of digits backward.

In computer-administered Digit Span tests, the digits are presented on a screen, which eliminates any verbal and reading bias. The ConductScience Digital Health mobile version of the test provides an engaging and easy-to-use platform to perform the Digit Span test.

Both options are available upon request: the ability for audible sequence priming or visual sequence priming with the ability for the respondent to answer with visual buttons on the screen.

Digit Span Test: Methods

The Digit Sequencing test measures one’s verbal working memory and attention. After the examiner has presented the digits, the participant must recall them – until they can’t remember the complete sequence, or until they repeat it incorrectly (Blackburn, 1957). Usually, the trials are presented twice.

The Digit Sequencing test can be administered backward, which means that the subject has to repeat the digits in reverse order. This variation requires the participant to hold the digits longer in their working memory. Thus, it’s believed that the ability to perform well on this additional difficulty of the task can be linked to general intelligence.

Note that the average score for adults is to recall between eight and ten digits forward and between seven and nine digits backward (Lezak, 2004).

Digit Span Test: Applications

The Digit Sequencing test has numerous applications in clinical practice and research.

Working memory. As mentioned above, the Digit Span is a great tool to measure working memory and verbal working memory, in particular. The test sheds some light on the process of encoding and receiving information, and also – on the way participants recall and verbalize their answers.

Overall intelligence. Originally designed as a part of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale, the Digit Sequencing test can indicate correlations between one’s working memory and overall intelligence. The extra effort required in the backward version of the test can be linked to intelligence.

Cognitive impairments. There’s a link established between the prefrontal cortex functions and working memory. MRI scans also indicate an activation of the prefrontal cortex during the test (Owen et al., 2000). Thus, the Digit Sequencing test can help researchers study patients with cognitive impairment.

Attention. The usage of the Digit Sequencing test to measure attention is a controversial topic. Initially, the test was designed to test working memory along with attention, as attention is a crucial aspect of one’s cognitive functioning and everyday life. Researchers have observed a decline in performance when one’s experiencing stress. Thus, it’s believed that there’s a link between the two (Lezak, 2004). However, research requires using other tools to measure attention (see Comparisons).

Digit Span Test: Comparisons

The Digit Sequencing test is usually given as a part of an assessment battery, such as the WAIS. The test has been adapted for adults and children.

When used as a tool to measure attention, the Digit Span tests should be combined with other measures. For instance, the Test of Everyday Attention (Robertson et al., 1996) is a reliable method, which consists of eight subtests, such as the Map Search subtest and the Elevator Counting with Distraction subtest. Note that the test-retest reliability coefficients for the Test of Everyday Attention are high (M= .7 for a control group and M= .78 for stroke patients). In addition, the test shows good face and convergent validity when combined with measures (Groth-Marnat & Baker, 2003).

Digit Span Test: Translations

The Digit Sequencing test includes only numbers, and as such, it’s been proven that the digit sequence has a greater advantage when compared to the non-digit sequence (Jones & Macken, 2015).

Digit Span Test: Results and Data Analysis

The digit sequencing test is better when compared with non-digit stimuli, such as letters (Jones & Macken, 2015). For instance, a study with 84 psychology students (age M= 22.07), conducted at the Nottingham Trent University, revealed the superiority effect of the digit sequence. Numerous studies show that the digit span increases with age.

What’s more, the digit sequence has been linked to instance-based associative learning (Jones & Macken, 2015).

Also, it’s believed that STM influences intelligence. In fact, results show that people with higher IQ score better.

Last but not the least, disorders, such as language impairments, may lead to low performance and scores on digit span tasks.

Figure 1 shows that younger adults perform better than their elderly counterparts in both forward and backward spans.

 
COSMIN

Generally speaking, the Digit Sequence has the highest specificity of all WAIS-III subtests (Groth-Marnat & Baker, 2003).

Internal reliability: The Digit Sequence tests show high internal reliability:.70-.90 (Conway et al., 2005).

Test-retest: The test-retest reliability is also good (.50-.70), and it can even reach .83 (Wechsler, 1997).

Validity: Nevertheless, the validity of Digit Sequence tasks can be questioned as there are many theories about the nature of memory, which makes it unclear what is being assessed. Note that the test reveals moderate criterion validity when correlated with the Stanford-Binet-IV composite score and Stanford-Binet-IV short-term memory; r = .48 and r = .52, respectively (Wechsler, 1997).

Digit Span Test: Strengths and Limitations

The Digit Sequence test is an effective method to measure verbal working memory.

However, more research is needed regarding various aspects, such as long-term associative knowledge, and the application of digit span tests with unfamiliar consequences in real life (Jones & Macken, 2015).

Nevertheless, the digit sequence shows a superiority effect when compared to any non-digit span, and the Digit Span test is a preferred method to measure one’s cognitive functioning.

In addition, computer and mobile versions of the test eliminate examiner differences and increase the inter-rater reliability (Collum et al., 2007).

Digit Span Test: Summary and Key Points

 
  • The Digit Sequence test is one of the main tools developed to measure one’s verbal working memory, overall intelligence, attention, and cognitive functioning.
  • The test was originally designed as an integrated part of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale.
  • During the standard test, the examiner reads a list of numbers, and the participant repeats them.
  • In computer-administered tests, the digits are presented on a screen, which eliminates any verbal and reading bias and reduces inter-rater variability.
  • We provide an engaging and easy form of the test.
  • In addition, the Digit Sequence test can be administered backward, which requires from the subject to repeat the digits in reverse order.
  • The test continues until the subject can’t recall the complete sequence, or until they repeat it incorrectly (Blackburn, 1957).
  • The Digit Sequence test reveals good psychometric properties, such as high internal reliability and test-retest stability.
  • The digit sequence has a superior effect when compared to non-digit tests. Results show that the digit span increases with age and correlates with intelligence.
  • The Digit Sequence test is an effective way to explore the labyrinths of people’s memory.

References

  1. Blackburn, H. &. (1957). Revised Administration and Scoring of the Digit Span Test. Journal of Consulting Psychology, Vol. 21, No. 2, 139-143.
  2. Collum, R., Rex, J. E., & Haier, R. (2007). General Intelligence and Memory Span: Evidence for a common neuroanatomic framework. Cognitive Neuropsychology, Vol. 24, No 8, 867-878.
  3. Conway, A., Kane, M., Bunting, M., Hambrick, Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, Vol. 12, No. 5, 769-786.
  4. Goldstein, E. B. (2011). Cognitive Psychology. Belmont, Ca: Wadsworth.
  5. Groth-Marnat, G., & Baker, S. (2003). Digit Span as a Measure of Everyday Attention: A Study of Ecological Validity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Vol. 97, 1209-1218.
  6. Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 1-13.
  7. Lezak, M. D. (Ed.). (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  8. Lovett, M. C., Daily, L. Z., & Reder, L. M. (2011). A Source Activation Theory of Working Memory: Cross-task Prediction of Performance in ACT-R. Washington DC: Air Force Office of Scientific Research.
  9. Owen, A., Lee, A., & Williams, E. (2000). Dissociating aspects of verbal working memory within the human frontal lobe: Further evidence for a “process-specific” model of lateral frontal organization. Psychobiology, 28 (2), 146-155.
  10. Robertson, I., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1996). The structure of normal human attention: The Test of Everyday Attention. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 2(6), 525-534.
  11. Wechsler, D. (1997). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-111. New  York: Psycho- logical Corp.

Create Digital Health applications

Contact Us