x
[quotes_form]

Number of Centre Crossings: A Sensitive Index of Exploratory Drive and Affective State in Open Field Analysis

Learn More about our Services and how can we help you with your research!

Introduction

In behavioral neuroscience, the Open Field Test (OFT) stands as one of the most established paradigms for quantifying spontaneous activity, risk assessment, and anxiety-related behavior in rodent models. It is a crucial assay for researchers aiming to understand how internal emotional states interact with spatial cognition and environmental perception. The test arena, typically a large, enclosed square or circular field, presents a dichotomous space: a protected peripheral wall zone versus a conspicuous and unprotected center zone. Rodents, as prey animals, exhibit innate avoidance of open areas, making transitions into the center a behavioral indicator of exploration versus fear.

Within this framework, the number of center crossings—counting how often an animal enters and exits the central zone—emerges as a powerful and underutilized measure. Unlike raw center dwell time or distance traveled, which can mask behavioral subtleties, crossing frequency captures dynamic movement patterns that reflect moment-to-moment decision-making. These transitions highlight the tension between exploratory drive and aversive avoidance, offering a sensitive index of emotional reactivity, cognitive flexibility, and locomotor coordination.

As a count-based metric, center crossings are particularly amenable to standardization, comparison across cohorts, and integration with both temporal and spatial behavioral features. They are influenced by pharmacological modulation, stress exposure, genetic background, and environmental context—making them highly valuable for both basic and translational research aimed at uncovering mechanisms of affective and neuropsychiatric disorders.

This count-based variable is defined as the total number of times a rodent crosses into the central zone (typically the inner 25% of the arena) from the periphery and exits back out. A higher number of crossings generally indicates greater willingness to explore and a lower anxiety profile, while fewer crossings suggest either avoidance behavior or reduced motivation.

What Does Number of Centre Crossings Measure?

Center crossings are a hybrid metric that encapsulate both locomotor dynamics and emotional decision-making. Each time a rodent enters or exits the central zone, it signals a behavioral choice—engaging with or retreating from an area that is evolutionarily perceived as vulnerable and potentially threatening. Thus, the frequency of crossings reflects the animal’s moment-to-moment balance between exploratory drive and anxiety-driven avoidance.

Frequent center crossings are generally interpreted as:

  • Indicators of low anxiety or high novelty-seeking behavior, where the animal demonstrates a willingness to enter and re-enter the exposed center.

  • Evidence of behavioral flexibility, showing that the animal is not perseverating in any one zone but is instead actively sampling the environment.

  • A proxy for sustained locomotor engagement, suggesting intact motor coordination and motivation.

In contrast, infrequent or absent crossings may imply:

  • Thigmotaxis-dominant behavior (persistent wall-hugging), often interpreted as heightened anxiety or defensive behavior.

  • Freezing, bradykinesia, or stress-induced inhibition, where the animal avoids locomotion altogether or limits its spatial transitions.

  • Reduced arousal, sedation, or sensorimotor deficits, potentially due to pharmacological side effects or neurodegenerative pathology.

What makes this metric particularly valuable is its ability to capture subtle changes in behavioral reactivity over short timeframes. Because center crossings reflect discrete transitions rather than continuous presence, they allow researchers to examine dynamic behavioral shifts that occur as an animal processes novel cues, habituates to the environment, or responds to pharmacological or genetic manipulations.

This measure becomes especially powerful when analyzed in conjunction with temporal segments (early vs. late behavior), paired metrics (e.g., time spent in center, latency to entry), and across conditions. It is not only a reflection of zone preference, but also a quantifiable index of risk evaluation, exploratory initiation, and motivational state. in identifying changes in moment-to-moment risk tolerance, especially in studies evaluating the acute impact of stress, pharmacological treatments, or genetic modifications.

Behavioral Significance and Experimental Relevance

The number of center crossings provides a real-time behavioral snapshot of how rodents resolve the competing motivations of safety and exploration. Unlike static measures such as time spent in specific zones, crossing frequency reflects active decision-making, locomotor persistence, and responsiveness to environmental cues. This measure is particularly suited for distinguishing between transient behavioral responses and persistent affective states.

1. Exploratory Pattern and Locomotor Strategy

High-frequency center crossings often correlate with structured exploration and effective environmental scanning. These animals exhibit a loop-like movement pattern—transitioning from wall to center and back—demonstrating both curiosity and spatial planning. In contrast, reduced crossings suggest a more restricted or perseverative movement strategy, often linked to anxiety or compulsive behavior.

2. Anxiety and Risk Assessment

Rodents inherently perceive the center zone of an open field as a high-risk area. As such, the number of center crossings is inversely correlated with anxiety-like behavior (Prut & Belzung, 2003). Animals with elevated anxiety or exposed to stressors (e.g., restraint, predator odor) show fewer crossings and increased wall-zone dwell time.

3. Cognitive Flexibility and Decision-Making

Frequent transitions between zones require cognitive engagement and adaptability. Genetic or lesion models involving the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, or amygdala often show altered crossing behavior, reflecting disrupted executive function or emotional regulation. This metric can therefore be used as a proxy for evaluating neural circuitry involved in affective-cognitive integration.

4. Pharmacological Modulation

The number of center crossings is highly sensitive to pharmacological interventions that alter anxiety, motivation, or arousal. Increases in crossing frequency following administration of anxiolytic agents (e.g., diazepam, fluoxetine) typically reflect reduced avoidance behavior and greater behavioral flexibility. In contrast, compounds that reduce central nervous system arousal, such as sedatives or high doses of antipsychotics, suppress crossings due to reduced locomotor output or impaired motor coordination. This makes center crossings a dual-purpose marker—sensitive to both emotional state and motor capacity—and supports its use in compound screening, drug safety evaluation, and behavioral pharmacodynamics.

Anxiolytic agents such as benzodiazepines, SSRIs, and GABA agonists typically increase center crossings, reinforcing the measure’s sensitivity to emotional modulation. Conversely, sedatives or motor-impairing drugs reduce crossings, highlighting the importance of interpreting this metric alongside total distance traveled and movement velocity.

Methodological Considerations

  • Zone Definition and Software Precision: Defining the central zone consistently is foundational for accurate crossing measurement. The zone is typically designated as the inner 25% of the arena, and this should be maintained across experiments for valid cross-subject comparisons. Advanced behavioral tracking software should be configured to distinguish complete, full-body entries and exits from edge-hovering behavior or partial zone overlaps. Irregular or oversized zone definitions can inflate crossing frequency and compromise interpretability.
  • Session Standardization: Uniform trial duration (commonly 5–10 minutes) is critical to ensure comparability of crossing frequency between animals and groups. Sessions that are too short may underestimate exploratory engagement, while longer sessions may introduce fatigue, satiety effects, or behavioral habituation. Standardizing start conditions, such as time of day or acclimatization period, further reduces session-to-session variability.
  • Animal Handling and Habituation: Rodent behavior is highly sensitive to pre-test handling and environmental novelty. To minimize stress-induced variability, researchers should standardize animal handling protocols and allow sufficient habituation to the testing room and apparatus prior to data collection. Pre-exposure to the arena (without data recording) can also reduce novelty-induced freezing and enhance exploratory validity.
  • Data Synchronization and Video Verification: Automated tracking should be complemented with synchronized video footage to confirm behavioral interpretations. This is particularly important when animals exhibit ambiguous behavior near zone boundaries, such as grooming at the center edge or circling with partial entry. Manual review ensures that counts reflect meaningful exploratory transitions.
  • Control for Locomotor Confounds: The number of center crossings is influenced by baseline activity levels. Animals with motor impairments, sedation, or low arousal states may show reduced crossings unrelated to affective state. Including complementary metrics (e.g., total distance traveled, movement velocity) helps control for confounding factors and ensures accurate interpretation of crossing data.
  • Environmental Consistency: Lighting conditions, arena geometry, ambient noise, and odor cues all impact exploratory behavior. Uniform lighting should minimize shadows or glare that may attract or repel movement. Between-session cleaning with scent-neutral agents ensures that olfactory residue does not bias zone preference or movement trajectories. Clearly define center zone boundaries using software that supports high-resolution spatial tracking. Misclassified entries or exits can inflate or suppress counts.

Integrating with Complementary Metrics

A comprehensive interpretation of center-crossing data is best achieved by combining it with additional spatial and temporal metrics. These complementary variables help distinguish between nuanced behavioral profiles, clarify the underlying causes of high or low crossing frequency, and rule out potential confounds related to general activity or motor capacity.

  • Time in Centre: This static measure offers critical context to crossing frequency. An animal with many crossings but low total time in the center may be exhibiting impulsive or indecisive behavior—frequently sampling the zone but retreating quickly due to anxiety. In contrast, high time in the center with few crossings may reflect sustained occupancy and reduced threat perception. Analyzing both metrics helps distinguish brief, exploratory entries from confident center engagement.
  • Latency to First Centre Entry: This measure captures the animal’s initial emotional response to the open field. A long delay before the first center entry may reflect high anxiety or environmental apprehension. When paired with low crossing frequency, it suggests consistent avoidance; when followed by high-frequency crossings, it may indicate rapid habituation or risk-reward re-evaluation.
  • Distance Travelled in Centre: This metric provides a spatial intensity layer. High center distance with few crossings may reflect long-duration center dwelling or circular exploratory paths, while low distance and low crossings suggest minimal interaction with the zone. Used in tandem, these metrics reveal whether entries are active and investigative or passive and brief.
  • Thigmotaxis Time: Time spent along the walls or corners of the arena inversely correlates with center activity. High thigmotaxis paired with few center crossings supports a strong anxiety-based avoidance profile. This relationship is especially valuable in anxiety or fear-conditioning studies.
  • Total Distance Traveled and Movement Velocity: Crossing frequency must be contextualized within overall locomotor output. Low crossings in the context of low total movement may indicate sedation, fatigue, or injury rather than emotional avoidance. Conversely, high crossings with high velocity may reflect hyperactivity or stimulant drug effects.
  • Rearing and Grooming Behavior: These micro-behaviors provide insight into stress response and environmental processing. High rearing near zone transitions may precede entries, while grooming during crossings may suggest stress-induced displacement activity. Their inclusion strengthens interpretation and behavioral modeling.

 

By triangulating the number of center crossings with these complementary indicators, researchers can form a multidimensional understanding of rodent behavior—uncovering latent patterns, resolving conflicting signals, and improving the interpretive resolution of Open Field data. Crossing frequency adds dynamic context to static duration measures. High time in center with few crossings may reflect passive dwelling, while high crossings with short center duration suggest impulsivity or hyperactivity.

  • Latency to First Centre Entry: A short latency paired with high crossing frequency supports a confident and exploratory profile; long latency with low crossings suggests sustained anxiety.
  • Distance Travelled in Centre: When paired with crossings, this measure helps differentiate active exploration from repetitive or stereotypic movement.
  • Thigmotaxis Time: High wall-time and low center crossings support an interpretation of strong avoidance behavior.

Applications in Translational Research

  • Psychopharmacology: The number of center crossings serves as a sensitive, quantifiable behavioral endpoint for evaluating the efficacy and side effects of psychoactive compounds. In early-phase drug screening, anxiolytics such as benzodiazepines, SSRIs, and GABAergic modulators often increase crossing frequency, whereas sedatives and psychostimulants may suppress or amplify this behavior depending on dose and mechanism of action. The dynamic nature of crossings allows the detection of both immediate and delayed drug effects, supporting its utility in pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic profiling.
  • Neurodevelopmental Models: Center crossing patterns can reveal key phenotypic traits in models of autism spectrum disorder, schizophrenia, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and intellectual disability. These models often exhibit deficits in spatial navigation, novelty preference, and sensory integration, which are reflected in abnormal crossing behavior. Quantifying these patterns can help validate animal models and align them with human symptom domains.
  • Stress and Resilience Studies: The ability of animals to maintain or recover exploratory behaviors following stress exposure is central to the study of resilience. Repeated assessments of center crossings before, during, and after stress-induction paradigms (e.g., chronic mild stress, social defeat, predator scent) allow researchers to monitor adaptive versus maladaptive trajectories. Increased crossings post-stress are often interpreted as signs of behavioral recovery or pharmacological rescue.
  • Aging and Neurodegeneration: Declining center crossings over time can serve as an early behavioral biomarker of age-related cognitive and motor decline. In Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s, or Huntington’s models, reduced crossing frequency may reflect spatial rigidity, impaired motor planning, or diminished novelty-seeking. These deficits are often observed before severe clinical symptoms emerge, making this metric valuable for preclinical intervention studies.
  • Endocrine and Hormonal Modulation: Center crossings are influenced by hormonal states such as estrous cycle phases, adrenal steroid levels, and thyroid function. Hormonal manipulations in rodent models—through gonadectomy, hormone replacement, or stress-hormone infusions—can significantly alter crossing frequency, revealing neuroendocrine contributions to anxiety and motivation.
  • Environmental Enrichment and Deprivation: Housing conditions such as social isolation, environmental complexity, or rearing enrichment significantly modulate exploratory drive. Animals raised in enriched environments often display higher center crossing rates, reflecting increased affective flexibility, while those raised in impoverished settings show reductions consistent with anxiety and reduced plasticity. Center crossings thus serve as a behavioral readout for affective and cognitive enrichment interventions. Used in screening for anxiolytic efficacy, center crossings provide an early and sensitive indicator of behavioral modulation.

Advanced Interpretive Dimensions

Center-crossing behavior is a rich, multidimensional indicator of how rodents engage with a test environment under different internal and external conditions. This metric not only reflects overt activity but encodes subtle affective and cognitive shifts that emerge as animals navigate competing motivational forces such as novelty, fear, and curiosity. The following dimensions offer deeper layers of insight into what center crossings can reveal when analyzed beyond raw counts:

Temporal Distribution of Crossings

Tracking center crossings over discrete time intervals within the session (e.g., by minute) reveals temporal exploration patterns. Animals showing an increasing trend across time may be exhibiting habituation and rising confidence, while those with early peak crossings that decline may display novelty-seeking followed by fatigue or anxiety relapse.

Zone Transition Sequences and Path Complexity

The order and pattern in which an animal moves between zones—e.g., from wall to center to corner—can offer deeper insights into locomotor strategy and emotional regulation. Combining center crossings with entropy or spatial complexity metrics supports more nuanced behavioral phenotyping, especially in models with repetitive behaviors or cognitive deficits.

Environmental Salience and Context Shifts

Center crossing frequency is sensitive to contextual cues such as lighting, ambient sound, or olfactory stimuli. Altering these environmental parameters can modulate the animal’s perception of safety, thus affecting transition rates. This responsiveness makes center crossings an ideal metric in environmental enrichment or contextual fear paradigms.

Inter-individual Variability and Subgroup Analysis

Not all animals exhibit the same center crossing patterns under identical conditions. By identifying subgroups—e.g., high-frequency explorers vs. low-frequency avoiders—researchers can uncover trait-level behavioral tendencies and predict responses to treatment, genetic modification, or stress exposure.

Enhancing Experimental Validity

The number of center crossings offers a highly accessible yet informative measure of emotional, cognitive, and locomotor integration. It captures exploratory transitions in real time and is sensitive to both subtle and overt behavioral changes. When combined with complementary spatial and temporal metrics, it enables high-resolution profiling of rodent behavior in the Open Field Test.

Explore high-resolution tracking solutions and open field platforms at

References

  • Prut, L., & Belzung, C. (2003). The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463(1–3), 3–33.
  •  
  • Seibenhener, M. L., & Wooten, M. C. (2015). Use of the open field maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments, (96), e52434.
  • Crawley, J. N. (2007). What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice. Wiley-Liss.
  • Carola, V., D’Olimpio, F., Brunamonti, E., Mangia, F., & Renzi, P. (2002). Evaluation of the elevated plus-maze and open-field tests for the assessment of anxiety-related behavior in inbred mice. Behavioral Brain Research, 134(1–2), 49–57.

Written by researchers, for researchers — powered by Conduct Science.

Author:

Louise Corscadden, PhD

Dr Louise Corscadden acts as Conduct Science’s Director of Science and Development and Academic Technology Transfer. Her background is in genetics, microbiology, neuroscience, and climate chemistry.