Introduction
The Open Field Test (OFT) is a foundational assay in behavioral neuroscience used to assess locomotion, anxiety-like behavior, and exploratory drive in rodent models. While commonly measured variables include total distance traveled and time spent in the center or periphery, Time in the Corners offers an additional, highly informative metric that reflects a subject’s shelter-seeking tendencies, risk assessment behavior, and emotional regulation.
In many rodents, corners represent the most secure locations within an open field due to the dual-wall configuration, providing tactile feedback and minimizing visual exposure. Prolonged occupancy of these zones can therefore be interpreted as a behavioral proxy for hypervigilance, passive avoidance, or contextual fear—core elements in the evaluation of anxiety and related affective states.
What Does Time in the Corners Measure?
Time spent in the corners of an open field arena captures the animal’s preference for the most spatially constrained and protected regions, offering a more refined view of risk-avoidance behavior than general peripheral movement. The dual-wall boundary of corners provides maximal tactile feedback and minimal exposure, triggering shelter-seeking tendencies especially in situations perceived as threatening or unfamiliar.
This metric integrates several behavioral constructs:
- Acute Stress Reactivity: Time in corners increases markedly in response to immediate stressors such as loud noise, bright light, handling, or novel environments. Animals instinctively retreat to corners as an emergency coping response—making this behavior a real-time indicator of acute anxiety.
- Chronic Emotional States: In models of chronic stress, depression, or neurodegeneration, elevated corner time may reflect long-term behavioral suppression, cognitive inflexibility, or generalized anxiety. This chronic pattern distinguishes corner occupancy from transient startle or exploratory hesitation.
- Conflict Between Safety and Exploration: The corners represent a spatial endpoint in the exploratory gradient of the open field. An animal that remains confined to corners is not simply anxious, but may be caught in a motivational stalemate between the desire to explore and the impulse to avoid risk. This ambivalence is key to understanding affective and decision-making circuits.
- Social and Environmental Context Sensitivity: Corner time is highly sensitive to contextual cues. Animals tested alone, under bright lights, or in olfactorily novel arenas tend to increase their corner preference. This makes the metric valuable for detecting subtle emotional shifts in response to environmental manipulations.
- Behavioral Stability and Trait Anxiety: Because corner time is relatively stable across repeated sessions in some animals, it serves as a behavioral marker for trait anxiety. When combined with other assays, this measure contributes to cross-test consistency in emotional profiling.
As a metric, time in the corners expands the behavioral resolution of open field testing. It captures subtleties of emotional withdrawal, sensory threat processing, and shelter-driven motivation that are often missed by broader categories like center or periphery time alone.. Unlike general thigmotaxis (wall-following behavior), corner-seeking represents an even more extreme form of avoidance, often signaling:
- Heightened anxiety or fear
- Shelter-seeking in response to environmental stressors
- Lack of exploratory motivation or behavioral inhibition
This metric is particularly useful in distinguishing nuanced behavioral phenotypes. For instance, two animals may spend equal time in the periphery, but only one may concentrate its activity in the corners—an important distinction when interpreting anxiety-like behavior.
Behavioral Significance of Corner Occupancy
Corner time reflects a complex interplay of cognitive, affective, and motivational processes that provide nuanced insight into the animal’s behavioral state:
- Passive Coping Strategy: Prolonged immobility in corners is often interpreted as a passive coping response to perceived threat. This posture minimizes movement and exposure, characteristic of emotional shutdown observed in models of learned helplessness and depression. It contrasts with active coping behaviors like center exploration or rearing.
- Spatial Safety Preference: Corners provide the most secure positions in an open arena, combining tactile support from dual walls with visual concealment. Animals with heightened threat sensitivity or hypervigilance naturally gravitate toward these regions, using them as defensive shelters. This spatial anchoring is particularly salient in models of PTSD, where exaggerated threat perception leads to excessive cornering.
- Reduced Exploratory Drive: High corner occupancy often reflects a broader motivational deficit. Animals that remain confined to corners typically exhibit reduced rearing, center transitions, or novelty engagement. This behavioral stagnation can stem from emotional suppression (e.g., anxiety, anhedonia) or neurocognitive factors like rigidity and inflexibility, especially in aging or neurodegenerative models.
- Environmental Familiarity and Adaptation: Over repeated testing or prolonged exposure, time in the corners typically decreases as animals habituate to the arena. This reduction in corner time is interpreted as diminished arousal and increased cognitive comfort with the environment. Therefore, corner metrics can be used longitudinally to track adaptation, resilience, or sensitization to contextual threat.
Relevance in Behavioral Neuroscience
1. Anxiety and Fear-Based Models
Corner time is elevated in rodent models of anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and generalized fear. Exposure to acute stressors (e.g., predator odor, bright lighting) or chronic stress paradigms (e.g., maternal separation, social isolation) increases time spent in the corners. Anxiolytic drugs often reduce this metric in parallel with center zone occupancy, reinforcing its relevance as an anxiety-sensitive endpoint (Prut & Belzung, 2003).
2. Depression and Behavioral Despair
Rodents displaying depressive-like phenotypes—such as anhedonia, psychomotor slowing, or behavioral suppression—frequently adopt corner-dwelling as a low-energy, low-risk posture. Increased corner time in these models may reflect emotional disengagement or impaired motivation to explore.
3. Neurodevelopmental and Aging Studies
Time in the corners is also informative in developmental and aging research. Juvenile animals tend to explore more freely and spend less time in the corners compared to aged or neurologically impaired counterparts. In neurodegenerative models, corner preference may signify cognitive inflexibility, risk aversion, or spatial disorientation.
4. Sex Differences and Hormonal Modulation
Hormonal influences, particularly estrogen and corticosterone, can modulate shelter-seeking behavior. Female rodents may exhibit cycle-dependent changes in corner preference, and sex-specific effects are commonly observed in response to anxiolytic and antidepressant treatments.
5. Pharmacological and Genetic Screening
Time in the corners provides a quantifiable endpoint for evaluating the effects of novel compounds or gene knockouts. For example, knockout models of serotonin transporter or corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) receptors exhibit elevated corner occupancy, linking this behavior to underlying neurotransmitter dysregulation.
Methodological Considerations
- Arena Design and Corner Geometry: Ensure the arena has clearly defined corners—this is typically achieved in square or rectangular arenas. The physical prominence and accessibility of corners can influence behavior, as sharper corners are more attractive for shelter-seeking than rounded or ambiguous ones. Avoid placing objects, shadows, or reflective surfaces in corners that might artificially inflate or deflate occupancy time. Consistency in arena design across trials is critical to maintaining data integrity.
- Automated Tracking Systems: Use high-resolution behavioral tracking software that allows for precise zone segmentation, especially when corners are defined as small regions. Corner zones must be consistently delineated using the same dimensions and coordinates across sessions. Automated scoring reduces observer bias, enhances reproducibility, and allows for temporal breakdowns of corner occupancy—such as frequency, latency to entry, and duration per visit.
- Session Length: Sessions lasting 5–10 minutes typically offer sufficient behavioral resolution for corner time analysis. Shorter sessions may miss important behavioral transitions, while longer sessions can introduce fatigue, decreased novelty salience, or increased freezing. Optimal session duration balances novelty-driven exploration with emotional and cognitive stability.
- Lighting and Sensory Cues: Lighting intensity and uniformity strongly influence corner preference. Uneven lighting can make specific corners more or less attractive, skewing results. Use diffuse, moderate-intensity illumination to avoid shadows and glare. In addition, control for ambient noise and residual scent cues between trials by using neutral cleaning agents and sound-attenuating chambers when possible. These steps ensure that observed corner preferences reflect emotional behavior rather than sensory confounds. Corners should be clearly defined and unobstructed. Arena shape (square vs. circular) directly influences the availability and salience of corners.
- Automated Tracking Systems: Use behavioral software that enables zone segmentation with high spatial resolution. Ensure that corner zones are consistently mapped across trials.
- Session Length: Sessions of 5–10 minutes typically provide sufficient data for corner analysis while avoiding fatigue or habituation effects.
- Lighting and Sensory Cues: Ensure consistent lighting to avoid confounding sensory influences on spatial preference. Reduce ambient noise and odor contamination between trials.
Interpretation and Integration with Other Metrics
Corner Avoidance vs. Corner Occupancy: Behavioral Subtypes
While increased time in the corners is often interpreted as an anxiety-like response, some animals may actively avoid the corners altogether. Distinguishing between these behavioral subtypes—corner-seeking versus corner-avoidant—adds granularity to phenotypic analysis. Animals that remain in the corners may exhibit passive coping or fear-based sheltering, whereas those that avoid corners might display hypervigilance, impulsivity, or heightened arousal.
Temporal Patterns of Corner Occupancy
Analyzing how corner time fluctuates during early, middle, and late stages of the test session provides deeper insight into behavioral adaptation. Early session corner occupancy may reflect initial freezing or startle responses, while persistent occupancy suggests enduring anxiety or cognitive rigidity. Tracking this trajectory can help distinguish between trait-like avoidance and state-dependent emotional reactivity.
Corner Time as a Function of Arena Design
The geometry and size of the arena influence spatial preferences. For example, square arenas with sharply defined corners often elicit more corner-focused behavior than circular arenas where such zones are absent. Standardizing arena parameters and defining corner zones consistently across studies improves cross-laboratory comparability and data interpretation.
Ethological Context: Naturalistic Meaning of Corners in Rodents
In natural environments, corners are associated with concealment, nesting, and reduced exposure to predators. Interpreting corner preference through an ethological lens enriches the behavioral meaning of this metric. It frames corner time not solely as a sign of anxiety but also as a contextually appropriate safety strategy under perceived threat.
Corner Time as a Predictor of Response to Intervention
Baseline time in the corners can serve as a predictor of treatment responsiveness. Animals with higher initial corner occupancy may be more sensitive to anxiolytic drugs or environmental enrichment, whereas those with low corner time may require different therapeutic strategies. This predictive utility supports its role in pre-screening, treatment stratification, and personalized experimental designs.
Cross-Paradigm Validity: Correlating Corner Time with Other Tests
Corner time shows strong construct validity when compared to related anxiety assays such as the elevated plus maze, light-dark box, or novelty suppressed feeding. Animals with high corner occupancy often display similar avoidant tendencies across these paradigms. Correlating across tests supports the use of corner time as a generalizable trait marker for affective behavior.
Integration with Machine Learning for Behavioral Clustering
When combined with other behavioral features, corner time can be incorporated into unsupervised machine learning models (e.g., k-means clustering, PCA, t-SNE) to classify animals into distinct behavioral profiles. This approach is particularly valuable for high-throughput phenotyping in large cohorts and for identifying subpopulations that may respond differently to interventions.
Corner time should not be interpreted in isolation. For comprehensive behavioral analysis, consider the following:
- Total Distance Traveled: Low movement combined with high corner occupancy may indicate sedation or emotional suppression.
- Time in the Centre and Wall Zones: Corner time often correlates inversely with center exploration and overlaps with peripheral thigmotaxis. A shift from wall-following to corner-dwelling may signal escalating emotional avoidance.
- Rearing and Grooming Behavior: Reduced rearing and increased grooming in corners may reflect stress-induced displacement behaviors.
- Latency to First Corner Exit: Extended latency suggests reduced motivation or elevated perceived threat.
Applications in Translational Research
- Anxiolytic Drug Discovery: Corner occupancy serves as a sensitive behavioral endpoint in early-phase screening of compounds targeting anxiety disorders. Anxiolytic agents—such as benzodiazepines, SSRIs, or GABAergic modulators—often reduce the time rodents spend in the corners, suggesting improved emotional regulation and reduced avoidance behavior. Corner time is particularly valuable in dose-response experiments, helping define minimal effective concentrations and behavioral efficacy profiles.
- PTSD and Trauma Models: In post-traumatic stress disorder models, corner time is elevated due to heightened hypervigilance and persistent risk-avoidant behavior. Exposure to trauma-related cues or predator scent increases corner-seeking, mirroring avoidance symptoms in human PTSD. This makes the metric a strong behavioral analog for tracking stress reactivity and evaluating interventions aimed at restoring affective stability.
- Developmental Disorders: Rodent models of neurodevelopmental disorders—such as autism spectrum disorders, fetal programming, and early-life stress—often show altered corner occupancy. Elevated corner time may reflect impaired social exploration, heightened sensory sensitivity, or disrupted motivational drive. As an early indicator, this metric is useful for identifying behavioral abnormalities before the emergence of more complex cognitive deficits.
- Environmental Enrichment Studies: Environmental enrichment—including social housing, novelty exposure, and sensory stimulation—reduces corner-seeking behavior over time. These changes reflect increased affective flexibility, resilience, and exploratory confidence. By quantifying shifts in corner time pre- and post-intervention, researchers can assess the impact of non-pharmacological therapies on emotional reactivity and adaptive behavior. Corner occupancy decreases following administration of effective anxiolytics, supporting its use in early-stage screening.
- PTSD and Trauma Models: Used to quantify hypervigilance and risk-avoidance behaviors following traumatic exposures.
- Developmental Disorders: Corner preference can serve as an early behavioral marker in models of autism spectrum disorders or fetal programming.
- Environmental Enrichment Studies: Reductions in corner time following enriched housing suggest improved affective flexibility and resilience.
Enhancing Research Precision with Standardized Systems
To accurately assess corner occupancy, researchers should employ standardized open field systems with reliable tracking capabilities and configurable zone mapping. High-resolution tracking allows for nuanced behavioral analysis, while data synchronization with video recordings enables post hoc validation. Ensuring consistent arena conditions, lighting, and habituation protocols improves reproducibility and supports more robust cross-study comparisons.
Explore the Open Field Test page for solutions designed to optimize behavioral testing and maximize insight into shelter-seeking and risk-assessment behaviors in animal models.
References
- Prut, L., & Belzung, C. (2003). The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463(1–3), 3–33.
- Seibenhener, M. L., & Wooten, M. C. (2015). Use of the open field maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments, (96), e52434.
- Crawley, J. N. (2007). What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice. Wiley-Liss.
- Cryan, J. F., Markou, A., & Lucki, I. (2002). Assessing antidepressant activity in rodents: recent developments and future needs. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 23(5), 238–245.
Written by researchers, for researchers — powered by Conduct Science.
See more of Our Posts
Behavioral Neuroscience

The Unseen Ritual: Understanding Grooming Behavior in Rodents

The Subtle Art of Togetherness: Exploring Social Preference in Zebrafish

The Hidden Patterns of Eating Behavior in Rodents: Insights into Neuroscience and Health

Twitching in Rodents: Understanding Motor Control, Sleep Physiology, and Neural Mechanisms
Sniffing in Rodents: Exploring Sensory Perception, Neural Mechanisms, and Genetic Influences

Drinking Behavior in Rodents: Mechanisms, Models, and Implications


Vertical Vigilance: Exploring Supported and Unsupported Rearing in Rodents

Walking in Rodents: Movement, Gait, and Motor Control

Why Do Rodents Jump? Exploring the Science Behind Their LeapsReferences
All Resources
Never miss our posts!
Author:

Louise Corscadden, PhD
Dr Louise Corscadden acts as Conduct Science’s Director of Science and Development and Academic Technology Transfer. Her background is in genetics, microbiology, neuroscience, and climate chemistry.