x
[quotes_form]
Automated Cell Counter

Ultimate Guide to Automated Cell Counter: Plus Purchasing Tips

Reference to this article: ConductScience, Ultimate Guide to Automated Cell Counter: Plus Purchasing Tips (2022). doi.org/10.55157/CS20220614

Introduction

Cell counting is routinely performed in life science research labs and medical diagnosis and treatment. There are many ways to count cells in labs, which do not require special equipment.

However, the use of equipment and instruments increases the accuracy of the obtained data. Additionally, they make the process faster and easier, even when working with more samples or needing to analyze large sample volumes.

The two categories of cell counting are manual counting and automated counting. The manual counting is done using a counting chamber or hemocytometer and plating or colony-forming unit (CFU) counting method.[1] In contrast, automated cell counting involves automated, high-throughput, specialized machines that automatically count the cells.

This article discusses the different automated cell counters, their principle and types, and how to choose the right automated cell counter for your lab.

What Is an Automated Cell Counter and How Does It Work?

An automated cell counter machine.

Figure: An image of an automated cell counter machine.

An automated cell counter is a machine that either works on the principle of image analysis or electrical impedance to count cells automatically.[2]

Principle of Automated Cell Counters

These cell counters are based on two basic principles: electrical impedance and the light scattering principle.[3]

1. Electrical Impedance Cell Counting Principle

Figure: An illustration of the electrical impedance principle of cell counting.[3]

The electrical impedance is also known as the Coulter principle.[3] Here, an increase in electrical resistance or impedance is observed each time a cell passes through an aperture similar to the size of cells present between two electrodes.

This change in impedance is directly proportional to the cell volume, which helps to count the number of cells in the sample’s given volume.[3]

2. Light-Scattering Cell Counting Principle

Figure: An illustrative diagram of the light-scattering cell counting experiment.[4]

The principle is based on the observation that cells, like blood cells, scatter into small angles when visible light is incident on them.[4]

Here, when a light beam is passed across a stream of diluted cells, it is deflected due to the cell, and the deflection is detected by the photodetector, which gives the number of cells present in the sample’s given volume.[4]

Types of Automated Cell Counter Methods

Based on different working principles, there are mainly four types of automated cell counter methods for different lab applications.[1]

1. Coulter Counter

In addition to determining the cell count, the device is also used to measure the cell volume in electrolytes. It is cheaper than a flow cytometer and has applications in particle characterization, hematology, and counting various cells, such as fat cells, plant cell aggregates, bacteria, and stem cell embryoid bodies.[5]

2. Image Analysis Method

This method involves using a microscope and statistical classification algorithm to carry out automated cell detection and then counting cells by image analysis.[1]

3. Flow Cytometry

Here, cells move in a narrow stream in the front of the laser beam, which, when hit, reflects the cells on a detector that keeps the cell count. This method is also used to analyze cell shape, their internal and external structures, and determine the number of proteins and certain biochemicals.[1]

Flow cytometry is the most expensive technique among automated cell counter techniques.

4. Stereological Cell Counting

In this method, cells are counted in histological sections. It utilizes a systematic and random sampling strategy to determine the frequency of objects or count the cell numbers.[1]

However, the technique is not fully automated as it involves manual decisions in sampling or including cells for the counting procedure and analysis.

Benefits and Limitations of Automated Cell Counters

Benefits

  • Cell counting is faster, statistically significant, and objective with an automated cell counter. It also doesn’t involve any distributional bias in manual cell counting.
  • The data produced are reliable and reproducible.
  • They are efficient and cost-effective when analyzing large amounts or numbers of samples.
  • They can perform multiple tests on a single platform.
  • The use of automated cell counters reduces labor to a large extent.[6]

Limitations

  • Some automated cell counters falsely increase or decrease cell counts. They might not be able to differentiate between nucleated red blood cells and small clumps of platelets.
  • The automated cell counters have high running costs.
  • They make workflow expensive.[6]

How to Use Automated Cell Counters and Prepare Samples

Automated cell counter machines work differently. So, it’s highly recommended to go through the user manual that comes with the commercial instruments to learn about their workings and procedures they can perform.

Below is the workflow for the most commonly used commercial automated cell counters:

The sample for cell counting can either be prepared without or with trypan blue staining.[7]

  • When preparing samples without trypan blue, directly pipette around 10 µl of cell suspension into the outer opening of the counting slide chambers.
  • When preparing samples with trypan blue, mix 10 µl of cell suspension with 10 µl of trypan. Then, pipette around 10 µl of the mixture into the outer opening of the counting slide chambers.
  • Insert the slide into the slide slot present in the automated cell counter machine.
  • The machine automatically starts counting the cell numbers in the given volume of the sample and provides a value in total cell count per ml.

If you observe “value out of range” on your screen, view the image captured and provided by the machine and determine if you need to dilute the cell suspension.[7] Once done, remove the counting slides from the slot and dispose of them as biohazardous waste.

Factors to Consider Before You Make That Purchase

You’ve learned how a spectrum of automated cell counters is commercially available, working on different principles. So, how can you decide which one fits your needs? We’ve made this easy for you by compiling a list of factors that you can follow before acquiring an automated cell counter.

1. Cell counting accuracy

The size of the cell counting area and variation in the data due to multiple counters influence the accuracy of the counted cells. And it’s believed that a larger area of counted cells and more sophisticated algorithms work best as they enable the correction in the hardware inequality and reduce the data variability.[8] 

2. Simple user interface

The automated cell counters are available with different features, including a user-friendly interface and button operator interface.[9] So, pick a system that is easy for you to operate, has features suiting your requirements, and produces consistent and accurate results within a short time.

3. Reproducibility

When working on high-throughput research, you want your data to be reproducible. Look for an automated cell counter with a defined measuring area for the cell counts. This way, you will get equal sizes of the captured images, reducing or eliminating variations in the field of view.[8]

4. Accurate focusing

A cell suspension often consists of both living and dead cells because it’s quite impossible to remove the debris and dead cells during sample preparation completely. But when counting cells, you don’t want to count the dead cells as living cells.

To help with this, the automated cell counters distinguish the live cells from dead ones by black edges and white centers.[8] So when purchasing an automated cell counter, make sure it has the autofocusing technology and high camera resolution.

5. Declustering performance

Some cells remain as cluster cells during sample preparation, and during counting, the cell counters might count them as one cell. This can shake the accuracy and reliability of your result. So, look for those automated cell counters that come with integrated software to recognize clustered cells and count each cell in the cluster.[8]

6. Storage of the result and analysis

The storage and memory of the cell counters is one essential factor in making your counting process a sliding ride. If you need to run a large number of samples routinely, then it’s preferable to purchase counters with better storage features.[8] Also, check the number of results it can store at a time.

7. Reliable and right supplier

It’s essential to purchase your automated cell counter from a known supplier. This will ensure you have a system with updated, first-hand software that works efficiently,[9], and they should also offer repair services.

Moreover, experienced suppliers can often help you choose the right equipment, fitting your needs and budget.

8. Lab space

Automated cell counters are available in many forms, benchtop, hand-help, or bulky forms. So, if you have constrained space, look for counters that take less space but have all the features you need to facilitate your process.

9. Cost/budget

What’s your assigned budget for the cell counter? Also, consider the prices of accessories or consumables of the equipment, such as disposable slides. Based on that, look for a supplier who can help you avail the equipment in your budget with effective maintenance services and service contracts.

Conclusion

Automated cell counters are instruments used in life sciences labs to count cells. It works on the principle of image analysis or electrical impedance to count the number of cells in the given sample.

The automated cell counters provide many benefits over manual ones, such as faster and more accurate results, less labor work, running multiple samples, and data storage. However, there’re also limitations to the machine, including the inability to differentiate dead cells from live cells and counting a cluster of cells as individual cells.

Different automated cell counters have different working principles and thus have different features and operational capabilities. Therefore, it’s necessary to consider machine features, storage, lab space, user interface, accurate focusing and cell counting, and budget when purchasing automated cell counters.

Our automated cell counter has got you covered if you need a faster and more efficient way to count cells from a reliable supplier at an affordable cost.

References:

  1. Cell counting. Retrieved from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Cell_counting
  2. Types of Automated Cell Counters. Retrieved from https://www.bio-rad.com/en-in/applications-technologies/types-automated-cell-counters?ID=LUSOMAOZR
  3. Electrical Impedance Methodology. Retrieved from https://hematologyacademy.com/hematologymethod/electrical-impedance-methodology/
  4. Principles of automated blood cell counters. Retrieved from https://clinicalsci.info/principles-of-automated-blood-cell-counters/
  5. Coulter counter. Retrieved from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Coulter_counter
  6. Advantages and Disadvantages of Automated Cell Counter. Retrieved from https://quizlet.com/77921079/advantages-and-disadvantages-of-automated-cell-counter-flash-cards/
  7. Manual vs. Automated Cell Counting. Retrieved from https://chemometec.com/resources/mini-reviews/manual-vs-automated-cell-counting/
  8. What to look for in an automated cell counter. Retrieved from https://www.westburg.eu/blog/cell-counter-tips
  9. How to choose a hematology cell counter. Retrieved from https://www.chinacaremedical.com/blog/how-to-choose-haematology-cell-counter_b0019.html

Learn More about our Services and how can we help you with your research!

Introduction

In behavioral neuroscience, the Open Field Test (OFT) remains one of the most widely used assays to evaluate rodent models of affect, cognition, and motivation. It provides a non-invasive framework for examining how animals respond to novelty, stress, and pharmacological or environmental manipulations. Among the test’s core metrics, the percentage of time spent in the center zone offers a uniquely normalized and sensitive measure of an animal’s emotional reactivity and willingness to engage with a potentially risky environment.

This metric is calculated as the proportion of time spent in the central area of the arena—typically the inner 25%—relative to the entire session duration. By normalizing this value, researchers gain a behaviorally informative variable that is resilient to fluctuations in session length or overall movement levels. This makes it especially valuable in comparative analyses, longitudinal monitoring, and cross-model validation.

Unlike raw center duration, which can be affected by trial design inconsistencies, the percentage-based measure enables clearer comparisons across animals, treatments, and conditions. It plays a key role in identifying trait anxiety, avoidance behavior, risk-taking tendencies, and environmental adaptation, making it indispensable in both basic and translational research contexts.

Whereas simple center duration provides absolute time, the percentage-based metric introduces greater interpretability and reproducibility, especially when comparing different animal models, treatment conditions, or experimental setups. It is particularly effective for quantifying avoidance behaviors, risk assessment strategies, and trait anxiety profiles in both acute and longitudinal designs.

What Does Percentage of Time in the Centre Measure?

This metric reflects the relative amount of time an animal chooses to spend in the open, exposed portion of the arena—typically defined as the inner 25% of a square or circular enclosure. Because rodents innately prefer the periphery (thigmotaxis), time in the center is inversely associated with anxiety-like behavior. As such, this percentage is considered a sensitive, normalized index of:

  • Exploratory drive vs. risk aversion: High center time reflects an animal’s willingness to engage with uncertain or exposed environments, often indicative of lower anxiety and a stronger intrinsic drive to explore. These animals are more likely to exhibit flexible, information-gathering behaviors. On the other hand, animals that spend little time in the center display a strong bias toward the safety of the perimeter, indicative of a defensive behavioral state or trait-level risk aversion. This dichotomy helps distinguish adaptive exploration from fear-driven avoidance.

  • Emotional reactivity: Fluctuations in center time percentage serve as a sensitive behavioral proxy for changes in emotional state. In stress-prone or trauma-exposed animals, decreased center engagement may reflect hypervigilance or fear generalization, while a sudden increase might indicate emotional blunting or impaired threat appraisal. The metric is also responsive to acute stressors, environmental perturbations, or pharmacological interventions that impact affective regulation.

  • Behavioral confidence and adaptation: Repeated exposure to the same environment typically leads to reduced novelty-induced anxiety and increased behavioral flexibility. A rising trend in center time percentage across trials suggests successful habituation, reduced threat perception, and greater confidence in navigating open spaces. Conversely, a stable or declining trend may indicate behavioral rigidity or chronic stress effects.

  • Pharmacological or genetic modulation: The percentage of time in the center is widely used to evaluate the effects of pharmacological treatments and genetic modifications that influence anxiety-related circuits. Anxiolytic agents—including benzodiazepines, SSRIs, and cannabinoid agonists—reliably increase center occupancy, providing a robust behavioral endpoint in preclinical drug trials. Similarly, genetic models targeting serotonin receptors, GABAergic tone, or HPA axis function often show distinct patterns of center preference, offering translational insights into psychiatric vulnerability and resilience.

Critically, because this metric is normalized by session duration, it accommodates variability in activity levels or testing conditions. This makes it especially suitable for comparing across individuals, treatment groups, or timepoints in longitudinal studies.

A high percentage of center time indicates reduced anxiety, increased novelty-seeking, or pharmacological modulation (e.g., anxiolysis). Conversely, a low percentage suggests emotional inhibition, behavioral avoidance, or contextual hypervigilance. reduced anxiety, increased novelty-seeking, or pharmacological modulation (e.g., anxiolysis). Conversely, a low percentage suggests emotional inhibition, behavioral avoidance, or contextual hypervigilance.

Behavioral Significance and Neuroscientific Context

1. Emotional State and Trait Anxiety

The percentage of center time is one of the most direct, unconditioned readouts of anxiety-like behavior in rodents. It is frequently reduced in models of PTSD, chronic stress, or early-life adversity, where animals exhibit persistent avoidance of the center due to heightened emotional reactivity. This metric can also distinguish between acute anxiety responses and enduring trait anxiety, especially in longitudinal or developmental studies. Its normalized nature makes it ideal for comparing across cohorts with variable locomotor profiles, helping researchers detect true affective changes rather than activity-based confounds.

2. Exploration Strategies and Cognitive Engagement

Rodents that spend more time in the center zone typically exhibit broader and more flexible exploration strategies. This behavior reflects not only reduced anxiety but also cognitive engagement and environmental curiosity. High center percentage is associated with robust spatial learning, attentional scanning, and memory encoding functions, supported by coordinated activation in the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and basal forebrain. In contrast, reduced center engagement may signal spatial rigidity, attentional narrowing, or cognitive withdrawal, particularly in models of neurodegeneration or aging.

3. Pharmacological Responsiveness

The open field test remains one of the most widely accepted platforms for testing anxiolytic and psychotropic drugs. The percentage of center time reliably increases following administration of anxiolytic agents such as benzodiazepines, SSRIs, and GABA-A receptor agonists. This metric serves as a sensitive and reproducible endpoint in preclinical dose-finding studies, mechanistic pharmacology, and compound screening pipelines. It also aids in differentiating true anxiolytic effects from sedation or motor suppression by integrating with other behavioral parameters like distance traveled and entry count (Prut & Belzung, 2003).

4. Sex Differences and Hormonal Modulation

Sex-based differences in emotional regulation often manifest in open field behavior, with female rodents generally exhibiting higher variability in center zone metrics due to hormonal cycling. For example, estrogen has been shown to facilitate exploratory behavior and increase center occupancy, while progesterone and stress-induced corticosterone often reduce it. Studies involving gonadectomy, hormone replacement, or sex-specific genetic knockouts use this metric to quantify the impact of endocrine factors on anxiety and exploratory behavior. As such, it remains a vital tool for dissecting sex-dependent neurobehavioral dynamics.
The percentage of center time is one of the most direct, unconditioned readouts of anxiety-like behavior in rodents. It is frequently reduced in models of PTSD, chronic stress, or early-life adversity. Because it is normalized, this metric is especially helpful for distinguishing between genuine avoidance and low general activity.

Methodological Considerations

  • Zone Definition: Accurately defining the center zone is critical for reliable and reproducible data. In most open field arenas, the center zone constitutes approximately 25% of the total area, centrally located and evenly distanced from the walls. Software-based segmentation tools enhance precision and ensure consistency across trials and experiments. Deviations in zone parameters—whether due to arena geometry or tracking inconsistencies—can result in skewed data, especially when calculating percentages.

     

  • Trial Duration: Trials typically last between 5 to 10 minutes. The percentage of time in the center must be normalized to total trial duration to maintain comparability across animals and experimental groups. Longer trials may lead to fatigue, boredom, or habituation effects that artificially reduce exploratory behavior, while overly short trials may not capture full behavioral repertoires or response to novel stimuli.

     

  • Handling and Habituation: Variability in pre-test handling can introduce confounds, particularly through stress-induced hypoactivity or hyperactivity. Standardized handling routines—including gentle, consistent human interaction in the days leading up to testing—reduce variability. Habituation to the testing room and apparatus prior to data collection helps animals engage in more representative exploratory behavior, minimizing novelty-induced freezing or erratic movement.

     

  • Tracking Accuracy: High-resolution tracking systems should be validated for accurate, real-time detection of full-body center entries and sustained occupancy. The system should distinguish between full zone occupancy and transient overlaps or partial body entries that do not reflect true exploratory behavior. Poor tracking fidelity or lag can produce significant measurement error in percentage calculations.

     

  • Environmental Control: Uniformity in environmental conditions is essential. Lighting should be evenly diffused to avoid shadow bias, and noise should be minimized to prevent stress-induced variability. The arena must be cleaned between trials using odor-neutral solutions to eliminate scent trails or pheromone cues that may affect zone preference. Any variation in these conditions can introduce systematic bias in center zone behavior. Use consistent definitions of the center zone (commonly 25% of total area) to allow valid comparisons. Software-based segmentation enhances spatial precision.

Interpretation with Complementary Metrics

Temporal Dynamics of Center Occupancy

Evaluating how center time evolves across the duration of a session—divided into early, middle, and late thirds—provides insight into behavioral transitions and adaptive responses. Animals may begin by avoiding the center, only to gradually increase center time as they habituate to the environment. Conversely, persistently low center time across the session can signal prolonged anxiety, fear generalization, or a trait-like avoidance phenotype.

Cross-Paradigm Correlation

To validate the significance of center time percentage, it should be examined alongside results from other anxiety-related tests such as the Elevated Plus Maze, Light-Dark Box, or Novelty Suppressed Feeding. Concordance across paradigms supports the reliability of center time as a trait marker, while discordance may indicate task-specific reactivity or behavioral dissociation.

Behavioral Microstructure Analysis

When paired with high-resolution scoring of behavioral events such as rearing, grooming, defecation, or immobility, center time offers a richer view of the animal’s internal state. For example, an animal that spends substantial time in the center while grooming may be coping with mild stress, while another that remains immobile in the periphery may be experiencing more severe anxiety. Microstructure analysis aids in decoding the complexity behind spatial behavior.

Inter-individual Variability and Subgroup Classification

Animals naturally vary in their exploratory style. By analyzing percentage of center time across subjects, researchers can identify behavioral subgroups—such as consistently bold individuals who frequently explore the center versus cautious animals that remain along the periphery. These classifications can be used to examine predictors of drug response, resilience to stress, or vulnerability to neuropsychiatric disorders.

Machine Learning-Based Behavioral Clustering

In studies with large cohorts or multiple behavioral variables, machine learning techniques such as hierarchical clustering or principal component analysis can incorporate center time percentage to discover novel phenotypic groupings. These data-driven approaches help uncover latent dimensions of behavior that may not be visible through univariate analyses alone.

Total Distance Traveled

Total locomotion helps contextualize center time. Low percentage values in animals with minimal movement may reflect sedation or fatigue, while similar values in high-mobility subjects suggest deliberate avoidance. This metric helps distinguish emotional versus motor causes of low center engagement.

Number of Center Entries

This measure indicates how often the animal initiates exploration of the center zone. When combined with percentage of time, it differentiates between frequent but brief visits (indicative of anxiety or impulsivity) versus fewer but sustained center engagements (suggesting comfort and behavioral confidence).

Latency to First Center Entry

The delay before the first center entry reflects initial threat appraisal. Longer latencies may be associated with heightened fear or low motivation, while shorter latencies are typically linked to exploratory drive or low anxiety.

Thigmotaxis Time

Time spent hugging the walls offers a spatial counterbalance to center metrics. High thigmotaxis and low center time jointly support an interpretation of strong avoidance behavior. This inverse relationship helps triangulate affective and motivational states.

Applications in Translational Research

  • Drug Discovery: The percentage of center time is a key behavioral endpoint in the development and screening of anxiolytic, antidepressant, and antipsychotic medications. Its sensitivity to pharmacological modulation makes it particularly valuable in dose-response assessments and in distinguishing therapeutic effects from sedative or locomotor confounds. Repeated trials can also help assess drug tolerance and chronic efficacy over time.
  • Genetic and Neurodevelopmental Modeling: In transgenic and knockout models, altered center percentage provides a behavioral signature of neurodevelopmental abnormalities. This is particularly relevant in the study of autism spectrum disorders, ADHD, fragile X syndrome, and schizophrenia, where subjects often exhibit heightened anxiety, reduced flexibility, or altered environmental engagement.
  • Hormonal and Sex-Based Research: The metric is highly responsive to hormonal fluctuations, including estrous cycle phases, gonadectomy, and hormone replacement therapies. It supports investigations into sex differences in stress reactivity and the behavioral consequences of endocrine disorders or interventions.
  • Environmental Enrichment and Deprivation: Housing conditions significantly influence anxiety-like behavior and exploratory motivation. Animals raised in enriched environments typically show increased center time, indicative of reduced stress and greater behavioral plasticity. Conversely, socially isolated or stimulus-deprived animals often show strong center avoidance.
  • Behavioral Biomarker Development: As a robust and reproducible readout, center time percentage can serve as a behavioral biomarker in longitudinal and interventional studies. It is increasingly used to identify early signs of affective dysregulation or to track the efficacy of neuromodulatory treatments such as optogenetics, chemogenetics, or deep brain stimulation.
  • Personalized Preclinical Models: This measure supports behavioral stratification, allowing researchers to identify high-anxiety or low-anxiety phenotypes before treatment. This enables within-group comparisons and enhances statistical power by accounting for pre-existing behavioral variation. Used to screen anxiolytic agents and distinguish between compounds with sedative vs. anxiolytic profiles.

Enhancing Research Outcomes with Percentage-Based Analysis

By expressing center zone activity as a proportion of total trial time, researchers gain a metric that is resistant to session variability and more readily comparable across time, treatment, and model conditions. This normalized measure enhances reproducibility and statistical power, particularly in multi-cohort or cross-laboratory designs.

For experimental designs aimed at assessing anxiety, exploratory strategy, or affective state, the percentage of time spent in the center offers one of the most robust and interpretable measures available in the Open Field Test.

Explore high-resolution tracking solutions and open field platforms at

References

  • Prut, L., & Belzung, C. (2003). The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. European Journal of Pharmacology, 463(1–3), 3–33.
  • Seibenhener, M. L., & Wooten, M. C. (2015). Use of the open field maze to measure locomotor and anxiety-like behavior in mice. Journal of Visualized Experiments, (96), e52434.
  • Crawley, J. N. (2007). What’s Wrong With My Mouse? Behavioral Phenotyping of Transgenic and Knockout Mice. Wiley-Liss.
  • Carola, V., D’Olimpio, F., Brunamonti, E., Mangia, F., & Renzi, P. (2002). Evaluation of the elevated plus-maze and open-field tests for the assessment of anxiety-related behavior in inbred mice. Behavioral Brain Research, 134(1–2), 49–57.

Written by researchers, for researchers — powered by Conduct Science.