Description
Take advantage of Neuralynx, Ethovision Integration, SMS and Email integration with the Conductor Science Software. No I/O Boxes Required
See our FULL citation list
Apparatus and Equipment
Training Protocol
Carefully position the subject into the restraining chamber and give it ample time to adjust to the environment. Repeat this process as needed to ensure the subject is relaxed and stable before conducting the actual trial.
Position the subject in the restraining compartment and let it acclimate for 15 to 20 minutes. To enhance heat absorption, you may paint the test area of the tail black. Place the tail directly under the heat source, then start both the timer and the heat source simultaneously. Monitor the subject for signs of pain until the pre-set cut-off time, typically between 20 and 35 seconds, is reached. Conduct additional trials as needed, ensuring a minimum 5-minute break between each trial.
Haddadi et al. investigated the impact of various doses of carvacrol on pain responses in adult male Wistar rats. The study involved 6-OHDA-lesioned rats divided into four groups receiving carvacrol at doses of 25 mg/kg, 50 mg/kg, and 100 mg/kg, with an additional control group and a sham-operated group. Each rat underwent three trials of 15 seconds, where different sections of the tail were exposed to a heat stimulus. Compared to the sham group, the lesioned rats exhibited a notable reduction in tail flick latency. However, carvacrol treatment did not produce a significant change in tail flick latency.
Li et al. categorized rats into nine groups, each receiving an intraperitoneal injection of either 0.9% normal saline, “n” (at doses of 2.5 mg/kg and 5 mg/kg), “a” (at doses of 42, 84, and 168 mg/kg), or various combinations of “n” and “a” (e.g., 1.25 N + 21 A, 2.5 N + 42 A, 5.0 N + 84 A mg/kg; n=9, 9, and 8 respectively) before conducting the tail flick test. Tail flick latencies were measured at five 15-minute intervals during a 30-second trial. The findings demonstrated a dose-dependent effect of the drugs on tail flick latencies. Notably, the combination of the two drugs produced a more pronounced antinociceptive effect compared to the effects of nefopam alone at equivalent doses.
Nealon et al. used experimentally naïve adult male and female C57BL/6N mice and divided them into two diet groups. One group was maintained on a high-fat, energy-dense, while the other group was maintained on standard chow for 15-weeks before the testing and drug administration. The subjects were treated with 0 (saline), 1, 3, 10, and 30 mg/kg “m”and tested for 10 seconds in the tail flick test. A dose-dependent increase in tail withdrawal latencies was observed. A difference in the magnitude of responses was observed between the two sexes, with females on either diet showing less sensitivity to effects of “m”.
In the Tail Flick test, the time taken for a tail to withdraw in response to a radiant heat stimulus is recorded. Withdrawal latencies can be assessed for various segments of the tail. It is crucial to distinguish reflexive behavior from voluntary actions during the observation. If any ambiguous behavior is noted, the experiment should be repeated to accurately determine and document the withdrawal latency.
Strengths & Limitations
The Tail Flick test assesses the subject’s response to thermal nociception. Unlike other tests, such as the Hot Plate test and the Hargreaves test, the Tail Flick test does not expose the animal to a heated open field or concentrate the stimulus on the plantar surface. Instead, it measures withdrawal latencies by applying heat to different areas of the tail. This test is straightforward and easy to conduct, offering two variations to accommodate different research budgets.
In spinally transacted subjects, the tail flick response may be a spinal reflex rather than pain response (Irwin et al., 1951). Thus, consideration of the subject’s motor function (Chapman et al., 1985) is vital in achieving accurate results. The observer should be able to differentiate voluntary movement from pain reflex behaviors. Since Tail Flick test requires restraining, it is essential that the subjects are well accustomed to that process. Restraining may also cause anxiety-related behaviors. The cut-off time should not exceed 30 to 35 seconds to avoid unnecessary harm to the subject. It is important to minimize any external stimulus in the test arena to reduce their impact on the subject’s performance. Handling and consistency of the surgical process can also potentially influence tail withdrawal responses. The tail flick test may not be successful in distinguishing between opioid agonists and mixed agonist-antagonists. However, this can be overcome by using a cold-water Tail Flick test (Pizziketti et al., 1985).
Summary and Key Points
- Tail Flick test was first described by D’Amour and Smith in the year 1941.
- Tail Flick test is similar in concept to the Hargreaves test. The heat stimulus is directed on the part of the subject’s tail rather than the plantar surface.
- Tail Flick test requires restraining of the subject.
- Tail Flick test can be performed either using an analgesic meter or using a water bath.
- Painting the portion of the tail, to be tested, black allows greater absorption of the heat,
- Spinally transacted subjects may show spinal reflex behavior rather than that induced by pain.
- Subject’s motor functionality can also impact withdrawal responses.
References
Chapman CR, Casey KL, Dubner R, Foley KM, Gracely RH, Reading AE (1985). Pain measurement: an overview. Pain. 22(1):1-31.
D’Amour F. E., Smith D. L. (1941). A method for determining loss of pain sensation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 72, 74–79.
Deuis JR, Dvorakova LS, Vetter I (2017). Methods Used to Evaluate Pain Behaviors in Rodents. Front Mol Neurosci. 10:284. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00284
Haddadi H, Rajaei Z, Alaei H, Shahidani S (2018). Chronic treatment with carvacrol improves passive avoidance memory in a rat model of Parkinson’s disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 76(2):71-77. doi: 10.1590/0004-282X20170193.
Irwin S, Houde R.W, Bennett D.R, Hendershot L.C, Seevers M.H (1951). The effects of “m”e and “m”on some reflex responses of spinal animals to nociceptive stimulation. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 101(2):132-43.
Li Q, Zhuang Q, Gu Y, Dai C, Gao X, Wang X, Wen H, Li X, Zhang Y (2018). Enhanced analgesic effects of “n” in combination with “a” in rodents. Biomed Rep. 2018 Feb;8(2):176-183. doi: 10.3892/br.2017.1032.
Nealon CM, Patel C, Worley BL, Henderson-Redmond AN, Morgan DJ, Czyzyk TA (2018). Alterations in nociception and “m” antinociception in mice fed a high-fat diet. Brain Res Bull. 138:64-72. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2017.06.019.
Pizziketti RJ, Pressman NS, Geller EB, Cowan A, Adler MW (December 1985). “Rat cold water tail-flick: a novel analgesic test that distinguishes “o” agonists from mixed agonist-antagonists“. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 119 (1–2): 23–9. doi:10.1016/0014-2999(85)90317-6.